Dan Willingham’s excellent article Inflexible Knowledge:
The First Step to Expertise (American Educator, Winter 2002) opens with the
following question.
“So often, even if I inventively present new material or
emphasize applying the new knowledge in various situations, what I get back
from my students seems ‘rote’. Why is this? What can I do about it?”
If you’ve asked yourself this question as an educator, I
recommend reading the article in full. But here are the highlights!
Willingham defines three types of knowledge in his article:
rote knowledge, inflexible knowledge, and flexible knowledge. He begins by
distinguishing rote knowledge (parroting without any understanding) from
inflexible knowledge (a seeming inability to apply knowledge learned).
Willingham uses an example from the book Anguished English to illustrate
rote knowledge. A student, asked to define equator, writes “a managerie
lion running around the Earth through Africa”. The student has memorized not
even the right words, but sounds, such that ‘imaginary line’ has turned into
‘managerie lion’ perhaps subconsciously associating Africa with the king of the
beasts.
Much more often, we encounter the situation where the
student can correctly produce a newly learned definition but seems unable to
use it. According to Willingham, “cognitive science has shown us that when new
material is first learned, the mind is biased to remember things in concrete
forms that are difficult to apply to new situations.” This is inflexible knowledge
and, Willingham argues, an important first step in the learning process. Why
does this happen? If something is unfamiliar, we automatically consider surface
features first checking if they resemble anything else we already know.
Abstracting the deep features from the surface features is
what distinguishes the expert from the novice. This is flexible knowledge – the
ability to apply our understanding of the deeper structure to a variety of
concrete situations, and not just the situation where the concept was first
learned. As educators, we want to move our students from surface-level thinking
to deeper thinking, leading them down the rabbit hole. You might think
that we can teach deep structure and abstraction directly to novice students,
but that turns out to hugely difficult. Willingham writes: “… cognitive
scientists have tried to [use it] many times. But, the problem with such direct
instruction is that the mind much prefers that new ideas be framed in concrete
rather than abstract terms.”
Reflecting on all of this reminded me of an office hour
session with one of my students three weeks ago. She told me she was having
trouble keeping straight all the different periodic table ‘trends’ we had
learned. When I asked her the definition of ‘Ionization Energy’ she struggled
to parrot the answer, at which point I asked her to stop and think about the
words ‘Ionization’ and ‘Energy’ meant. A moment later she was able to give me
the correct definition. But then she seemed to be guessing whether the first
ionization energy increased or decreased across a row or down a column, and
unable to use the definition to help her. I moved the conversation away from
ionization energy back to something she could more easily concretize – atomic
size. She could correctly tell me the size trends, and with a bit of delving,
eventually able to explain her answers in concrete terms. Then I asked her the
definition of ‘Effective Nuclear Charge’ and this time she was able to figure
it out and then use the concept to explain the size trends. At this point we
returned to ionization energy and she was now able to determine and explain the
trends. Success!
Next she asked me about electronegativity. I asked her for
the definition and got a garbled response. She looked up the definition and was
able to repeat it to me, but then together we parsed the definition carefully
to make sure she paid attention to all the attendant parts. I asked her how electronegativity
might connect to effective nuclear charge, and with a bit of (mental) prodding,
she could now explain the trend. To tie it together, I had her summarize all
three trends and connect them to each other. The whole process probably took at
least 30 minutes one-on-one. It takes time and energy for those disparate
pieces of information to click together as an integrated whole. In a classful
of students, I had actively connected the definitions and trends, and had the
students think-and-reason through the process. That may have clicked for some
students, but clearly it did not for others. In this one particular case, I
hope I successfully increased the student’s knowledge flexibility at least for
this one concept.
How might one overcome the bias to “remember things in
concrete forms that are difficult to apply to new situations”? Willingham
suggests it “seems best overcome by the accumulation of a greater store of
related knowledge, facts, and examples.” He ends the article with suggestions
for teachers.
(1) Use multiple examples and encourage students to see what
the examples have in common, preferably leading them to the deeper structure.
(2) Distinguish rote knowledge from inflexible knowledge,
not despairing from the latter, but using it as a building block to flexible
knowledge.
(3) Appreciate that learning facts can be helpful and
encourage students’ growing their knowledge even if it’s inflexible. “Knowing
more facts makes many cognitive functions (e.g., comprehension, problem
solving) operate more efficiently.”
The last point is important especially with the recent popularity
of emphasizing broad skills over facts/content in education circles. While we’d
like for students to acquire those deeper ‘critical thinking’ skills, the road
to it is paved with facts and content. Thanks to cognitive science research, we
now have good evidence that higher order abilities are domain-dependent. The
more you know about something, the better you are able to turn that knowledge
from inflexible to flexible. Content and critical thinking are not opposed to
each other. The more factual knowledge you have, the more able you can think
about the area critically.
No comments:
Post a Comment