In my most recent post, I mentioned a monograph I’ve been reading: Concepts of Matter in Science Education. One of the articles that made me really stop and think was by Vicente
Talanquer, titled: “How do students reason about chemical substances and
reactions?” How do they indeed? It turns out that they often rely on heuristics
– some of which mislead them. What are heuristics? Here’s how Talanquer
describes it from p340 of the monograph minus the many references. (If you’re
interested I encourage you to read the article in full.)
“Heuristic reasoning in judgment and decision-making has
been analyzed from a variety of research perspectives. Despite differences in
conceptualization and approach, existing frameworks highlight the capacity of
the human mind to make decisions with very little time and information, using
implicit and preconscious reasoning mechanisms. These types of reasoning
strategies have been characterized as fast and frugal because they employ a minimum
amount of time and information to generate a choice or decision and [are]
adaptive or ecologically rational because they fit to the structure of the in
which they are used. Heuristic processing can be expected to dominate over more
analytical ways of thinking when a person has less knowledge, capacity or
motivation to do well in a task. Although heuristics usually provide
satisfactory answers, they do not always lead to the correct solution and seem
to be responsible for many systematic biases and errors in human reasoning.”
Let’s look carefully at that second last sentence and apply
it to students. When a student has less knowledge or capacity, there is a
reliance on applying heuristics rather than more a careful analysis. That’s
certainly true in my experience. The less knowledgeable and less capable students
do provide heuristic type answers on an exam. Some of these have the “correct”
buzzwords but are used incorrectly, and I can tell that the student doesn’t
have a clear understanding of the concept being applied. Others are just plain
wrong. On the other hand, the students who have a stronger grasp on the
underlying concept are able to justify their arguments rationally and
logically.
But sometimes students who seem to be able in a class discussion
or in my office to make coherent, logical, rational arguments in answering a
conceptual question fail to do this on an exam. (When a student asks a
question, I usually don’t answer them directly, but rather provide them the
tools to formulate their argument through prompts.) I think this is where time
pressure can lead a student to fall back on a heuristic. I have timed exams in
the majority of my classes (although I have experimented with other open-ended
forms) because I think a time constraint does provide some measure of how well
the student actually knows the material. It’s not a perfect measure by any
means, and there are disadvantages to having timed exams. (Open-ended exams
however have a different set of disadvantages.) For that matter, using exams in
both teaching and assessment has its pros and cons. I think the pros outweigh
the cons in a number of chemistry courses especially in the first year of
college
Talanquer’s article makes reference to studies in first and
second year college chemistry (usually General Chemistry and Organic Chemistry)
whereby a large proportion of students “rely on heuristic strategies, rather
than analytical thinking based on atomic-molecular models of matter...
Heuristic reasoning allowed participants in our studies to reduce cognitive
effort by minimizing the number of cues that needed to evaluate to make a
decision.” This has echoes of cognitive load theory, and I think the cognitive load is particularly high in chemistry because of the back-and-forth between
atomic-particle level explanation and macroscopic observations mediated by a
symbolic language and a dizzying array of models. This problem in chemistry is
often referred to as the Johnstone Triangle.
A few heuristics are highlighted in the article. Recognition is used when an object is
recognized and exhibits the known property. The example provided is that
students often select NaCl as being more water-soluble than NaBr simply because
the first is more recognizable. One-reason
decision-making is the tactic of searching for a single differentiating cue
to answer a question. The example used is that the student might assume that
BaO has a higher melting point than MgO because Ba is “heavier” than Mg, and
simply stop there without any further analysis.
As an expert, I also use heuristics of a sort when thinking
about chemistry, but my knowledge base is much wider and deeper than the
student’s. Thus my recognition
heuristic isn’t limited to just a few representative cases, nor would I quickly
stop at a single differentiating cue or use a one-reason approach. The question is how to help my students move
towards a deeper analysis. Clearly widening and deepening the knowledge base is
important – and that is why content
is important in the study of chemistry. But the process of reasoning through
multiple factors can also be modeled in the classroom.
No comments:
Post a Comment