In an attempt to raise the level of intrinsic motivation, I
have experimented with de-emphasizing grades in a previous course, and I am
using part of that strategy in my general chemistry course this semester. I am
also reducing the contribution of exam grades to the course grade; also an
experiment.
Jack Michael thinks one should do the opposite, although he
restricts his suggestions to courses with more than 40 students,
introductory-content-heavy, and aimed at first and second year students. In a
provocative article from 25 years ago, he argues that “effective college
teaching is a form of aversive control, but if done properly the aversiveness
is quite mild, and such aversive control can be responsible for the development
of large and valuable intellectual repertoires.” That’s the last sentence of
the article abstract, shown in full in the picture below along with source
information.
I rarely have over 40 students in a class even at the
introductory level, so maybe none of this applies to me. However, while I don’t
necessarily agree with everything in the article, Professor Michael makes some
incisive and insightful points in his paper. It is well worth reading in full,
and I will simply highlight a few things that jumped out at me.
The first is his view of the introductory-content-heavy
“grunt” courses. He writes: “I know that some professors and some students
consider such courses to be necessary evils, at best, brought on by mass
education, but I don’t share that view. This type of course, when it generates
effective study, is responsible for a great deal of learning.” The key argument
that Michael makes is that for many areas, particular in the sciences and
courses leading to particular professions, having an extensive knowledge base
is crucial. Creativity and innovation flow from this base – and the student
learns from the professor (the professional in his or her field) how the expert
talks and thinks about such material. As a chemist, I am inclined to strongly
agree – I’ve started to entertain the idea of teaching a one-credit-hour
seminar on the deep structure of chemistry. Maybe that will be my next summer
project.
A chunk of the article discusses student motivation. The
author anticipates many of the arguments one would make about the different
sources of motivation. Intrinsic motivation – the joy of learning for its own sake
– which is what we would ideally want all our learners to have, unfortunately
plays a very small role and tends to be limited to a very narrow band of
students. Our accelerating milieu coupled with a range of competing interests
demanding the attention of our students, pushes students away from putting in
the time and effort needed in a college-level course. Even “long-range payoffs
suffer from the same susceptibility to postponement… this type of motivator
[doesn’t] play any appreciable role in maintaining daily and weekly study”.
What works? Michael’s answer: Grades. He calls it “the one
motivational factor over which the instructor has considerable control, and
which is easily related to the details of the study assignment. It is also a
factor of considerable strength, as evidenced by the intensity of study
activity occurring immediately before a major exam.”
This brings me to the Procrastination Scallop. I had read
about this in a different article that then referenced Jack Michael’s paper and
led me to it in the first place. His paper has only one diagram – and it’s
excellent! I should show it to my students.
He then goes into detail about how one should structure
exams, how often, how much weight they should be given, the importance of
clarity, the link to out-of-class study, and the relationship between student
mastery and grades. He does weekly exams – I’m not sure I could do that,
although I tend to give more exams than most. He also thinks that these exams
should contribute significantly to the course grade. That’s what I’ve done in
most of my classes (except for the experiment I’m trying this semester), which
now seems rather “old school” given newer, trendier approaches. But perhaps
there is something worthwhile about the old-school method. It’s something for
me to mull over at length.
I close this post with Michael’s debunking of three popular
notions in his conclusion. These notions are:
·
If you teach properly the students will find
learning both fun and easy.
·
Grades should not be emphasized.
·
Good teaching consists primarily of good
lecturing.
You’ll have to read his article to find out why he thinks
these are bad for the types of courses he is considering (see second paragraph
of this post). His final few sentences are poignant. “Perhaps in a more general
sense it would be useful to conclude as follows: The world runs on fear.
College learning is largely under aversive control, and it is our task to make
such control effective, in which case it becomes a form of gentle persuasion.”
The author doesn’t try to fight the system but operate as
best within it. Can we get out of the cycle of aversive control that is lodged into a much larger system? That will require more than a summer project of
thought.
No comments:
Post a Comment