In my previous post, I mentioned that I’ve been thinking
about how to factor in cognitive load as I design activities in my classroom.
My thinking was partly motivated by having just read a monograph by A.J. Martin
based on his Vernon-Wall lecture [1] in 2015. The title of his lecture: “Can
educators reduce students’ cognitive load and boost motivation and engagement?
Integrating explicit instruction and discovery learning through Load Reduction
Instruction (LRI).”
That’s quite a mouthful. Let’s break it down.
The goal of LRI is to optimize student learning and
achievement. The trick is to match the cognitive demands required with where
the students are developmentally in the learning process. In the early stages,
when students are novices, a more
structured approach (explicit instruction) that reduces cognitive load is
required. This is because the novice mainly resorts to working memory, limited
in its capacity. During this period, students gain fluency and build basic
content and skills into long-term memory. This frees up working memory for
further learning, and that’s when guided inquiry learning approaches can be a
stepping-stone to autonomous learning. (We want students to learn how to
learn!)
The focus on the article isn’t on achievement (there is a
summary of previous work and an extensive citation list) but instead explores
the effects of LRI on motivation and engagement. There is a Motivation and
Engagement Wheel that encapsulates the main concepts of the article. It resembles
a color wheel, except in the article I downloaded there was no color, just
shades of grey, perhaps appropriately. I’ve also included a color version I
found that uses traffic light colors from a quick Google search. Both are shown
below. There is a more extensively annotated version later in the article not
shown here. The monograph is dense with terminology but Martin clearly explains
all the different pieces. (That’s probably why it is 35 pages long!)
The monograph contains a number of examples and practical
suggestions included that illustrate the conceptual pillars of LRI. One section
that I found quite interesting was the discussion on the southeast corner of
the wheel. I had vague anecdotal notions of why my students would be anxious,
and how this could result in poorer performance, but no particular conceptual
framework to think about the issues involved. In a section on “Anxiety, failure
avoidance, and self-handicapping”, Martin outlines the sources and downstream
effects. Basically, anxiety, fear of failure, and other distractions, “may act
as a source of extraneous cognitive load and tap the limited capacity of
working memory”. These fears may also lead to self-defeating behavior,
procrastination being one among many examples. The LRI pedagogical strategy in
these cases is to reduce split-attention by carefully structuring the material
to reduce the load on working memory.
A significant chunk of the article is devoted to discussing
how LRI integrates both explicit instruction and inquiry-based learning, two
pedagogies that are often portrayed (falsely in my opinion) as being in
opposition to each other. The strawman in one corner is the “traditional
lecture” and in the other corner we have “completely open-ended discovery”.
These two approaches are philosophically rooted in positivist and
constructivist approaches respectively. There is an extensive review of this
debate in a book by Tobias and Duffy [2]. It prompted me to go back and re-read
an excellent article from the book authored by Paul Kirschner [3]. I highly
recommend it. The abstract is shown below.
Kirschner begins by defining epistemology and pedagogy. A
scientist’s epistemology might be the “scientific method” (in its various
guises). On the other hand, pedagogy refers to “strategies or styles of
instruction”. Importantly, pedagogies can be (a) general, (b) domain-specific,
or (c) specific to a certain approach to teaching. It is this third case that
has caused the most misunderstanding as different groups tout their superior
pedagogies. Three-letter acronyms are common, e.g., PBL = Problem Based
Learning, TBL = Team Based Learning, and there is also LRI mentioned above.
Kirschner quickly gets to the matter at hand. The most relevant section of the
article to scientist-educators is appropriately titled: “Practicing Science or
Learning to Practice Science?”
The movement in recent years has been towards constructivist
approaches. (It’s more like the swinging back and forth of a pendulum, but
discovery-based learning is what’s popular at the moment.) These voices declare
that “inquiry into authentic questions generated from student experiences
should be the central strategy for teaching science.” Kirschner quotes others:
“The ultimate goal was to provide a learning environment in which students
could feel like scientists in their own classrooms. This meant that our
students would need to be involved in the acquisition of their scientific
knowledge – not only reading and writing about – but actually doing science.”
As a chemist, I don’t disagree that students should be
exposed to a curriculum that involves lab-based hands-on exercises. These are
crucial to building important manual and observational skills, and can
complement what the students are learning in the “lecture” portion of the
course. However, touting the “doing science is the best way to learn science”
as the primary pedagogical driving
force at the introductory level is
what I find questionable. The most recent PISA results would seriously question
this credo, although oddly enough it hasn’t drawn much attention in the mass
media. Martin somewhat alludes to this in his article, but Kirschner tackles it
head on. I’m going to quote his clear prose [4].
“This focus is coupled to the assumption that to teach the
process of science (i.e., the pedagogy), we can best confront learners with experiences
either based on or equivalent to science procedures (i.e., the epistemology).
This has led to a tenacious commitment by educators, instructional designers,
and educational researchers to discovery and inquiry methods of learning which
is based upon confusing teaching science as
inquiry (i.e. an emphasis in the curriculum on the processes of science) with
teaching science by inquiry (i.e.
using the process of the science to learn science). The error here is that no
distinction is made between the behaviors and methods of the scientist – who is
an expert practicing her or his profession – and those of a student who is
essentially a novice.”
“A student, as opposed to a scientist, is still learning
about the subject area in question and, therefore, possesses neither the
theoretical sophistication nor the wealth of experience of the scientist. Also,
the student is learning science – as opposed to doing science – and should be
aided in her/his learning through the application of an effective pedagogy and
good instructional design.”
“[Discovery] presupposes a prior conceptual framework and
the ability to interpret and sometimes reinterpret what has been seen or
experienced in abstract terms, but there is no guarantee that it will lead to
new concepts, much less correct ones. This is because, first, novices have
little knowledge and experience in a domain which causes them to encode
information at a surface or superficial level… Second, novices do not simply
produce random guesses in the absence of knowledge, but rather as
systematically off the mark in a particular way that makes sense given a
particular misconception. The strangest and possibly most unfortunate aspect of
this whole problem is that it is not new…”
Kirschner argues that “the lack of clarity about the
difference between learning and doing science has led many educators to
advocate the discovery method as the way to teach science.” This fits well with
current trends. Kirschner’s article was 7-8 years ago. Constructivist
pedagogies have been further on the rise since. It is unfortunate that
“lecture” has been getting a bad rap. There is a place for lectures amongst the different pedagogies. Experienced educators in the
trenches will utilize a variety of pedagogies. You know what works, what
doesn’t, and you constantly try to hone your methods with each group of
students. What worked in the past may not work in the present. The problem is
when administrators or governing bodies demand a particular pedagogy be used
because it is deemed a “best practice”. In tertiary education, much autonomy
still rests with the professor. But in primary and secondary education, the
confusion between epistemology and pedagogy could result in some poor
practices. As the unbundling of education continues,
however, the problems may mount. We should be watchful, thoughtful and act accordingly.
[1] The Vernon-Wall lectures are part of the annual
conference of the Psychology of Education Section of the British Psychological
Society. The official citation for this monograph is: Martin A. J. (2016). Using Load Reduction Instruction (LRI) to
boost motivation and engagement. Leicester, UK: British Psychological
Society.
[2] Tobias, S. & Duffy, T. M. (Eds.) (2009). Constructivist Instruction: Success or
Failure? New York, NY: Routledge.
[3] P. A. Kirschner (2009). Epistemology or Pedagogy, That is the Question.
[4] I have left out the references and a number of
intervening paragraphs between the three quoted. I recommend reading the entire
article.
No comments:
Post a Comment