The Centre for Education Statistics & Evaluation at the
New South Wales (Australia) Department of Education recently released a short
primer summarizing Cognitive Load Theory. Here’s a link to the website with the document. The text itself is a short, readable, seven pages and hits the
main points in what I think is a fair and balanced summary.
Cognitive Load Theory came out from the work of John
Sweller. He has also written a comprehensive book, or you could consult the creative visual summary of each chapter by Oliver Calgiol at this blog. I’ve
blogged about this particularly where it pertains to the learning of chemistry. A one-sentence summary can be found in the second paragraph of the
primer: “Research in cognitive load theory demonstrates that instructional
techniques are most effective when they are designed to accord with how human
brains learn and use knowledge.”
The two foundational principles of cognitive load theory
are: (1) “there is a limit to how much new
information the human brain can process at one time”, and (2) “there are no
known limits to how much stored
information can be processed at one time.” The key distinction is new versus
stored information, or using working memory versus accessing long-term memory.
Thus learning is a process of turning new things processed in working memory
into schemas that “place” this new information in long-term storage in a way
that it can be retrieved for continued learning to take place – building one
block on top of another in a sense. However the limits of working memory mean
that it can be overloaded, resulting in non-learning or worse, mis-learning.
There are three types of “loads” contributing to the overall
cognitive load: intrinsic, extraneous and germane. Intrinsic load is “the
inherent complexity of the material and the prior knowledge of the learner”.
Extraneous load is “poorly designed instruction that does not facilitate
appropriate schema construction” while germane load is its opposite. The way
this translates into effective teaching is that at the introductory level
teaching should be explicit and less open-ended “discovery”, but as the learner
builds more complex schema, more student independence is built into the
assignments. There is an excellent article by Paul Kirschner, mentioned in a previous blog post, discussing the confusion facing educators in the
sciences between pedagogy and epistemology – confusing teaching science by inquiry versus teaching science as inquiry.
Several examples of research are provided that feed into
recommendations for the classroom. But the authors are careful to discuss the
relevance and limitations of their research in the final section of the primer.
“Cognitive load theory is particularly relevant to teaching novice learners in
so-called ‘technical’ domains such as mathematics, science and technology… Far
less research has been done on whether [it] is effective for teaching in less
technical, or more creative subjects areas – such as literature, history, art
and other humanities subjects.” In addition, “the literature on cognitive load
theory is also silent on how other factors besides cognitive load [such as
learner motivation] might influence the effectiveness of learning.” But it’s an
excellent primer overall, and well-worth the seven minutes it will take you to
read it.
No comments:
Post a Comment