Do children lose their creativity as they grow older? Are
present formal school systems responsible for suppressing the ‘natural’
creativity of kids?
Several authors in Creativity and Development suggest
that these questions are not meaningful because they fundamentally assume that
the answer to the question “Are children creative?” is Yes. But perhaps the
answer is No.
Here’s a provocative excerpt from David Henry Feldman, currently
a professor at Tufts, and one of the contributors.
I suppose it depends on how one defines ‘creativity’. The
contributors to Creativity and Development, all major figures in their
field who have pondered this issue, differ in specifics but they all
acknowledge that context is important. They try to counter the ‘myth’ that
creativity is primarily a function of the individual. Instead the environment,
society, field and domain play a part in whether some ‘thing’ will be
ultimately judged creative (with hindsight, of course). I purposefully kept ‘thing’
vague because while it is common to assess creativity via a product (a work of
art, an insightful theory, a nifty device), one could conceive a creative act
where the process is by far the crucial piece and not the product. Improv is
often cited as an example.
Why do we hearken to the idea of the creative child? Here’s
an excerpt of a thoughtful narrative by Seana Moran, currently a professor at
Clark University.
There is some correlation between child’s (fantasy) play and
standard tests of divergent thinking (one measure of creativity), but the
effect is small. Are there developmental precursors to adult creativity? That’s
one of the questions explored by the book – relating creativity to a
developmental process across time, challenging the popular trope of the eureka
moment. If our brains and thought processes develop differently over time, do
we need different theories of creativity for different life stages? Are
transitions between one stage and the next creative, akin to the emergence of a
phase transition on the natural sciences where something novel and impactful is
produced?
In Chapter 1 of the book, R. Keith Sawyer (also the volume editor)
explores the idea of emergence in the context of creativity and development.
Freud and Piaget make their appearance, but so do other thinkers. The broad
definition of creativity is introduced. “Creativity is a socially recognized
achievement in which there are novel products.” In Chapter 2, Seana Moran and
Vera John-Steiner analyze the contributions of Vygotsky to the dialectic of
creativity and development. I knew something of Vygotsky’s theories and targeting
the ‘zone of proximal development’ has been one of my teaching mantras for many
years. However, I learned much more about the context of Vygotsky’s ideas and
how they have (developmentally) influenced contemporary theories in the field.
I also appreciated Vygotsky’s emphasis on the joy of learning amidst the
struggle! I should consider how to incorporate this in my chemistry classroom.
Robert Sternberg, a stalwart in the field, discusses how
creativity with a small ‘c’ develops in the process of decision-making in
Chapter 3. I’ve read a lot of Sternberg so I didn’t find his chapter as novel-ly
interesting as the others but he provides clear theoretical models with
practical implications. He also has a nice section titled “Teaching Creativity:
21 Ways to Decide for Creativity” that I might assign my students to read in my
classes. I’ve been thinking about how to inject some type of Creativity-in-Chemistry
into my classes and experimenting in small doses with particular activities and
class projects.
The chapter I found most interesting (and I surprised myself
because I typically shy away from reading biographies) was David Henry Feldman’s
attempted reconstruction of the Multiple Intelligences (MI) Theory developed by
Howard Gardner. While I’m familiar with the populist view of MI Theory, I’ve
had a knee-jerk reaction against it because of how it has been misused in
popularizing Learning Styles (successfully debunked, yet lives on – like a
zombie). Reading the context in which Gardner developed his theory gives me a
greater appreciation for what he was trying to accomplish as a challenge to the
then-dominant and narrow approach of psychometrics. I also enjoyed reading
Chapter 5 (“Creativity in Later Life”) by Jeanne Nakamura and Mike
Csikszentmihalyi. As someone over-the-hill (i.e. on the other side of age
forty), I found it encouraging to ponder the many and varied examples from
interviews with folks in their sixties, seventies and eighties. There was the
joy of continuing to be creative coupled with a realistic appraisal of
additional constraints added by age.
As a book in the Counterpoint series, the authors discuss a
range of questions in the final chapter. These include the two quoted
screenshots above, but I want to share one more because it got me thinking
about my research into origin-of-life chemistry. This one is also by Seana
Moran.
I’m interested in proto-metabolism. How does a collection of
small molecules emerge into a primitive metabolic cycle that streamlines into a
novel energy transducing system that in one instance promotes an explosion of
diversity (think about the range of novel life forms!) with strong constraints
on the building blocks. In prebiotic chemistry, the problem is an embarrassment
of riches. How did nature prune its metabolic pathways to only use a small subset
amongst the myriad closely related chemicals? There are many different amino
acids, sugars, and pterins that could function similarly. Why did nature lead
to such a narrow selection? And yet the diversity of life-forms is fantastic.
Moran’s ecological approach gave me an outside-the-box idea of how to attack
this problem.
While novelty is often emphasized in creativity, constraints
provided by the environment might be equally important. The open-endedness of
an overly dilute soup of novelty might just as well be tons of uselessness.
While I may disagree with particular curricular choices of formal school
systems, the philosophy of having a formal school system may be good for a
creative society as a whole. It’s difficult to be creative in a domain and/or
field in which one has very little knowledge, and formal systems help to build
that knowledge base. On the other hand, as one gains expertise and is
acculturated into a community of practice, those constraints could also act as
blinkers that hinder subsequent creative acts. Striking the right balance is
tricky.
Reading Creativity and Development slowly over the
last two weeks has made me think seriously about putting together some broader Creativity-in-Chemistry
projects. I now have some vague outlines of how to proceed and I’m looking
forward to discussions with colleagues and students about these ideas before launching
a pilot program. I should also get back to working on my back-of-the-envelope chemistry card game. I developed it a little farther over Thanksgiving break so
I’m looking forward to winter break to refine my ideas!
No comments:
Post a Comment